Skip to main content

Children’s experiences of welfare in modern Britain: 5. ‘Everything was done by the clock’: agency in children’s convalescent homes, 1932–61

Children’s experiences of welfare in modern Britain
5. ‘Everything was done by the clock’: agency in children’s convalescent homes, 1932–61
    • Notifications
    • Privacy
  • Project HomeChildren’s Experiences of Welfare in Modern Britain
  • Projects
  • Learn more about Manifold

Notes

Show the following:

  • Annotations
  • Resources
Search within:

Adjust appearance:

  • font
    Font style
  • color scheme
  • Margins
table of contents
  1. Cover
  2. Half Title
  3. Title Page
  4. Copyright Page
  5. Contents
  6. Acknowledgements
  7. List of figures and tables
  8. List of abbreviations
  9. Notes on contributors
  10. Introduction
    1. Rethinking the history of welfare
    2. Approaches and sources
    3. Rethinking histories of modern Britain
  11. 1. Children’s experiences of the Children’s Friend Society emigration scheme to the colonial Cape, 1833–41: snapshots from compliance to rebellion
    1. The Children’s Friend Society and the Cape colony
    2. Letters home
    3. Scandals and silences
    4. Conclusion
  12. 2. ‘Their mother is a violent drunken woman who has been several times in prison’: ‘saving’ children from their families, 1850–1900
    1. ‘I determined to change my name and deny all knowledge of living relations’: children’s choices and their consequences
    2. ‘I shall always look on the time I spent at Waterlands as being the turning point of my life’: the importance of relationships in intervention
    3. Conclusion
  13. 3. ‘Dear Sir, remember me often if possible’: family, belonging and identity for children in care in Britain, c.1870–1920
    1. Creating an institutional ‘family’
    2. Maintaining family bonds
    3. Children’s responses to family practices
    4. Conclusion
  14. 4. Child philanthropy, family care and young bodies in Britain, 1876–1914
    1. Childhood in the public sphere
    2. Institutional care
    3. Parental and peer care
    4. Conclusion
  15. 5. ‘Everything was done by the clock’: agency in children’s convalescent homes, 1932–61
    1. Privacy
    2. Discipline
    3. Conclusion
  16. 6. ‘The Borough Council have done a great deal ... I hope they continue to do so in the future’: children, community and the welfare state, 1941–55
    1. Essay collections
    2. Desire for reform
    3. Living conditions
    4. Education
    5. Healthcare
    6. Conclusion
  17. 7. Welfare and constraint on children’s agency: the case of post-war UK child migration programmes to Australia
    1. The policy and organizational context of post-war UK child migration to Australia
    2. The nature and effects of constraints upon child migrants’ agency
    3. Learning from children’s experience of constraint in welfare services
    4. Conclusion: thinking about children’s experiences of agency in relation to welfare
  18. 8. ‘The school that I’d like’: children and teenagers write about education in England and Wales, 1945–79
    1. Child-centred buildings
    2. Teachers and power relationships
    3. The curriculum, age and child psychology
    4. Truancy and school refusal
    5. Conclusion
  19. 9. Making their own fun: children’s play in high-rise estates in Glasgow in the 1960s and 1970s
    1. High-rise, children and play
    2. Children’s play in Glasgow’s high-rise: Queen Elizabeth Square and Mitchellhill
    3. Where did children want to play?
    4. Memories of ‘living high’ – where did you play?
    5. Conclusion
  20. 10. Teenagers, sex and the Brook Advisory Centres, 1964–85
    1. Clients’ experiences of sexual services: the challenge of finding sources
    2. The Brook Advisory Centre and its clientele
    3. Clients’ lived experiences with the clinic
    4. Clients’ influence over the service
    5. Contraception and the under-sixteens
    6. Conclusion
  21. Postscript: insights for policymakers and practitioners
  22. Index

5. ‘Everything was done by the clock’: agency in children’s convalescent homes, 1932–61

Maria Marven

Earlier you said that your convalescent home was like a concentration camp, will you tell me what you meant?

Royston (1952, eight years old): Well, I didn’t mean literally, you know, just that it wasn’t, it was very stark and, em, regimented (pause). Oh, it was controlled, yeah, very controlling and everything was done by the clock, and, em, we were just processed along. But really, we were just like numbers to them – a job. There was no fun, em, just trudging along. But other times, well (pause) it wasn’t all bad, not really. You know, I was a bit of a live wire in them days, you know, a rebel, so they couldn’t keep me down! I can remember having fun and playing games, playing with the other kids. You know, really, we made our own fun. I’d say it was more like somewhere between Belsen and Butlins.1

In 1952, eight-year-old Royston was admitted to a convalescent home for two months while he recovered from surgery. He was one of the multitudes of children who were admitted to convalescent homes between 1845 and 1970. The great majority were working-class children who, it was believed, were in need of fresh air, good food and rest to recuperate from ill health and escape the corrosive effects of urbanization. Children’s institutional convalescence was part of a general mushrooming of voluntary healthcare provision that occurred during the nineteenth and twentieth centuries. Convalescent homes were sponsored by a range of individuals and organizations, but these can be broadly divided into six main categories: homes affiliated to a religious body; homes affiliated to a hospital; independently owned homes; homes owned by an existing charitable organization; local authority owned homes; and after 1948, National Health Service homes.

The motivations for establishing and sponsoring a children’s convalescent home varied between individuals, organizations, and over time. Sponsors were motivated by a number of factors that often coexisted, combining altruism, civic duty, religious obligation, self-interest and the objectives of the nation-state. As the motivations of sponsors evolved over time, so did the provision of homes. There were three cycles of expansion and contraction between 1850 and 1970, with two World Wars punctuating an overall pattern of growth between 1850 and 1955, followed by an eventual decline in the 1970s. Within each general growth cycle there were distinct differences in the growth patterns of various categories of homes. This reflected changes in the motivations of sponsors and their support for children’s institutional convalescence (Figure 5.1).

Figure 5.1. Number of convalescent homes that admitted London children by sponsorship type, 1850–1970.

Source: Data drawn from a number of directories: Burdett’s Hospital and Charities Directory, Church of England Yearbook, The Hospital Year Book, Kelly’s Directories, King’s Fund Directory of Convalescent Homes Serving Greater London, Low’s Handbook to the Charities of London.

Despite the durability of their association with children’s healthcare, scholarly investigation of children’s convalescent homes is sparse. Typically, historians have tended to research innovations in child health through the provision of community-based welfare clinics, domiciliary nurse visits and school medical inspections. The residential healthcare of sick children has received limited attention.2 Work by Harry Hendrick has begun the process of turning the focus of attention towards children’s hospitals.3 His work explores children’s experiences of hospitalization through medical case notes, doctors’ reports and official documentation; unfortunately, these sources do little to uncover the experiences of sick children.

Broader historical research on children’s residential institutions has demonstrated a wide range of provision and inmate experience. Much of this work suggests that these institutions shared a number of objectives. They sought to control and reform children who were perceived as problematic by society, including the poor, sick and disabled, commonly by their permanent removal from their familial homes.4 The dominant interpretation of children’s welfare institutions remains one of coercive, isolating and uncaring places, in which the discipline and punishment of children was often unnecessarily severe. Recent work has challenged this assessment and emphasized the extent to which regional contexts, subject populations and local officials varied the institutional experiences of children.5 Yet, despite this rich historiography, individual experience and day-to-day institutional policies and practices have too commonly been overlooked.

Since Roy Porter’s call to study history from below, historians have turned to new methods of analysis to study the subjective experiences of the non-hegemonic classes and subaltern groups. However, the voices of child patients have remained persistently silent. This chapter uses the remembered experiences of institutional convalescence as a prism through which to view children’s subjective experiences of healthcare. The close examination of fifty-three oral history interviews with individuals who had been admitted to convalescent homes between 1932 and 1961 allows those who experienced convalescence to be the central figures of this chapter. All of the interviewees lived in the London area at the time of their admission and characterized themselves as coming from working-class families. They were admitted to twenty-four different homes for a variety of medical and social reasons, and they stayed on average for four months.

Alessandro Portelli argued that oral history requires specific interpretive instruments that are different to written sources.6 The method of interpretation deployed in this study was to undertake an intensive analysis of each interview using psychologist Carol Gilligan’s Listening Guide method.7 The Listening Guide is a way of analysing qualitative interviews, drawing on the clinical methods of Freud, Breuer and Piaget. It is composed of a series of sequential readings, or ‘listenings’, that allow the researcher to uncover the varying voices of an interviewee. The Listening Guide is composed of four steps. The first step listens for the plot and the researcher’s response to the interview. The second listening focuses on what the Listening Guide calls the ‘I’ voice, by following the use of this first-person pronoun, and constructing an ‘I poem’. I poems pick up on an associative stream of consciousness carried by a first-person voice running through a narrative, rather than being contained by the structure of full sentences. In some cases, an individual’s I poem illuminated a theme that was not directly stated by the interviewee, but was central to understanding what was being said. An example of this can be seen in an extract from the I poem of Chris, and sheds light on why he repeatedly absconded from his convalescent home, even though he was considered to be a mature, sensible child. Chris was admitted to a home for four months in 1950, when he was ten years old.

I was the sensible one
I can’t imagine what possessed me
I was quite advanced, mum said more than my brothers
I knew what to expect
I was with a small group of boys
I didn’t mind because I understood
I was advanced for my age
I explained to the others
I went in first
I was put into the hands of a nurse
I tried to tell the nurses that
I couldn’t understand what was happening
I didn’t know what to do next
I just remember this, an overwhelming sense of confusion
I felt really lost
I didn’t know what to do
I had a wobbly
I ran
I wanted to get away
I just decided to run and so I ran8

The third step of the Listening Guide is shaped by the specific questions guiding the research. During this step, each transcript was read and simultaneously listened to, observing for a particular strand, or voice of the interviewee’s remembered experience. Gilligan described this as listening for ‘contrapuntal voices’. The final step of the Guide pulls together what has been learnt about the interviewee into a single analysis that can be used alongside their transcript. Taken as a whole, using the Listening Guide method facilitated a comprehensive analysis of all of the interviews that accommodated an awareness of subjectivities and composure of self-narratives.

Colin’s I poem illustrates that careful analysis of oral history testimonies can deepen our historical understanding of children’s dependence and interdependence and how they exercised agency in a diversity of ways. By considering the various ways that children enacted agency, this chapter complicates institutional narratives that identify children as homogenous, subordinate subjects. Instead, it positions children’s agency as shifting, negotiated exchanges shaped by age, context, relationships and cultural norms. Concepts of agency were conceived to account for the behaviour of adult, usually economically privileged, white males. Positioning children as independent social actors challenges historians to reconceptualize and broaden their definitions of agency. Mary Jo Maynes has observed that many of the ordinary understandings of agency and power simply do not apply to children.9 She suggests that by critically engaging with definitions of agency, the importance of age as a category of historical analysis comes sharply into focus. In problematizing definitions of agency, this chapter rotates around the key foci of privacy and discipline. The first section explores the various ways in which institutional practices challenged children’s privacy and provides a unique view of the cultural constructions of privacy. Children enacted privacy norms by deploying various behavioural devices to maintain their privacy; this provides valuable information regarding the significance of age to agentic behaviour. The second section examines discipline within children’s convalescent homes. The institutional discipline of children who, for the most part, were not perceived as problems, provides a deeper understanding of social attitudes towards discipline than is available from current scholarship’s focus on delinquents. The different modes of discipline used within convalescent homes, and their varying influence on an individual’s ability to exert agency, draw attention to the pernicious effect of isolation within institutional settings.

Privacy

Historians have recently begun to pay attention to the place of privacy in history. Their scholarship has emphasized the experience of adults and we know very little about the meanings of privacy to children.10 A richer body of scholarly work in the field of child psychology has addressed the privacy requirements of children.11 Psychologists have suggested that how children conceptualize privacy increases in complexity with age and an associated growing appreciation of themselves as social objects.12 In their oral history testimonies, respondents’ understandings of childhood privacy, and the extent to which it was met, challenged or encroached upon was strongly influenced by their age at admission. However, there was not an age-related linear progression of privacy needs, moving from low to high. Instead, there were often seemingly contradictory representations of childhood privacy, particularly in the areas of toileting, bathing and emotional privacy. By exploring each of these areas in turn, it is possible to observe the complexity of children’s privacy needs, and the extent to which age, context and relationships combined to influence an individual’s experiences and perceptions.

Attempts to maintain their privacy in the lavatory featured in the narratives of the great majority of interviewees. All of the interviewees were continent and able to use the lavatory independently at the time of their admission. Their familial homes had a variety of facilities consisting of: single household with outside toilet (28); single household with inside toilet (11); shared toilet with one other family (7); shared toilet with two other families (5); shared toilet with more than two other families (2); and a communal pot or bucket for night time use only (33). These various facilities were understood to be normal by respondents and usually only mentioned in response to a direct question. Conversely, lavatory facilities in convalescent homes and the difficulties they experienced maintaining personal privacy during elimination were proactively mentioned by eighty-seven per cent of respondents.

Their statements draw attention to a set of generalized, age-specific institutional regulations that governed children’s behaviour in the lavatory and inhibited the amount of privacy they were allowed. Individuals aged seven years and under at the time of their admission recalled that they were required to use a potty, rather than a lavatory. Between the ages of eight years and eleven years, children were permitted to use the lavatory, but were required to leave the door open and were closely supervised by staff. Children over the age of eleven years were permitted to shut the door, but not to lock it or flush the toilet. Older children were also required to gain permission from staff before going to the lavatory and request paper if required. Interviewees believed that these requirements challenged not only their need for privacy, but also their sense of maturity and their age-related identity. Typical in this regard were the experiences of Anthony and Bertha.

Anthony (1950, six years old): [T]‌he nurses would open, bring out these steel pans, you know, potties ... and arrange them in a grid (tapping table in a row) and then the children were assigned to a potty and, and as I recall there was no sex segregated, ... they told us basically to get, em, undressed and to go into those potties. And when I was told what I was expected to do in a potty, I was shocked, really horrified and, em, I was too old to use a potty, I’d been going to the proper toilet for years. I felt humiliated, to have to sit like a baby, in the presence of all those other children, not just boys, but girls too! It was terrible, really terrible.13

Bertha (1939, nine years old): [P]‌art of the system when a child went to the toilet, you couldn’t close the door and the nun stood and watched you. And I found that a terrible experience, a real sort of, I was a very private person, I was, I was private, and I didn’t want to see, them seeing me doing my business. (Pause) Another experience that sort of made it, at the time, painful.14

In common with all interviewees who recalled episodes in which their privacy was challenged, Anthony and Bertha narrated their experiences in the voices of despondency and anger. However, thirty-one of the respondents also recalled behaviour in which they sought to regain their privacy and exert control over their environment. When narrating these episodes, the voice of confidence came to the fore. The following extract from the testimony of Mavis illustrates the contrapuntal motion between the voices of despondency, anger and confidence. The counterpoint between these three voices demonstrates the complexity of children’s agency. In many instances it was the emergence of the voice of confidence that drew attention to agentic behaviour that may have otherwise been overlooked, as it frequently did not conform to adult patterns of behaviour. Mavis was admitted to a convalescent home for six months, suffering from tuberculous glands and weight loss.

Mavis (1936, six years old): I hardly know how to tell you this, but it’s been on my mind, and I just, it was a horrible experience. And I still remember it so clearly. (Pause) We had to, em, use the potty, not a, a proper toilet, a potty. I remember that it was, em, in a very large room, with possibly, maybe ten other children, and I didn’t want to take my knickers off, and it was just all very embarrassing. We were all sitting on potties, and we had to do what the nurses said ‘Do your duty’, I remember that phrase very well, ‘Do your duty’ (clears throat). And I remember trying to ask for the toilet, and getting so upset, very upset because I wasn’t allowed. They wouldn’t, they made me sit on the pot, but I refused to use it, because I was too old to sit on a potty, and well, to, to actually use it was (pause). So instead, instead I found a game that I’d play, a great game, it was a shiny floor and if I moved my feet backward and forward, I could slide over the floor, and I would try to move along the row while the nuns weren’t looking. And gradually other children joined, and I would lead the way, but in the end presumably they did their duty because I was the last one left with the nun.15

Mavis’ testimony demonstrates that children’s agentic behaviour differs to that of adults, and may be embedded within other activities, including play. Sociologist William Corsaro has shown that children use play to both reinforce and subvert institutional rules, and, like Mavis, use communal play as acts of subversion and to build peer culture.16 These modes of behaviour were also observable in the testimonies of older children, although they tended to deploy increasingly sophisticated modes of agentic behaviour. An example of this was the ‘fainting game’ recalled by Saul, in which a child distracted nursing staff by holding their breath and pretending to faint, thus allowing other children to ‘sneak in [the toilet] without being seen’ by nursing staff.17 This game is interesting because there was an acceptance that not all of the participants would immediately receive the benefit of privacy, and children ‘took turns to faint’. The complexity, trust and protracted nature of the fainting game demonstrates both the cohesiveness of peer group culture built through play and the degree to which peer group relationships supported autonomy and subversive resistance to authority.

Memories of play acts indicate that individuals used play to confront confusion and fears generated by institutional rules. Nine of the older children recalled singing or speaking very loudly to let staff and other children know that they were in the lavatory, or placing jumpers over their knees to ‘preserve [their] modesty’.18 This is what Corsaro calls a secondary adjustment, in which children use legitimate resources in artful ways to get around rules.19 Interviewees who were approaching adolescence, although given the greatest amount of privacy, adopted the most direct approaches in securing their privacy. However, this was never overt confrontation with staff; instead, artfulness or deceit were commonly employed. Pam was admitted to a convalescent home for three months for the treatment of her ‘nerves’. In her interview, she described her feelings of embarrassment and how she avoided asking for toilet paper.

Pam (1948, twelve years old): If you wanted the toilet you had to put your hand up and ask, and ask for toilet paper if you needed it. And I didn’t like this, I thought myself too grown up, you see, and to put your hand up and ask, and, having to announce that you wanted the toilet and needed paper was, was very embarrassing, especially at that age, and, well it seems a silly thing now, but it wasn’t then. I was very concerned about what people thought, and it was, em, embarrassing. ... [T]‌here was ways you soon learned, ways round it, by taking the paper without asking and sneaking to the loo when they weren’t watching (laughing), things like that.20

Respondents’ testimonies indicate that they understood themselves to be active participants in securing their privacy, but they exerted their agency in many different, distinctly age-specific, and often oblique, ways.

New and different patterns of age-related behaviours were observable when children sought to maintain their privacy during bathing. Only eight respondents’ familial homes had bathrooms; the great majority recalled a tin bath pulled into the kitchen and filled with water. Over ninety per cent of respondents recalled bathing at home between once and three times a week; only three respondents remembered baths as being rare events. For younger children, bathing was usually a communal activity with similar-aged siblings or even cousins. In some cases, this would be with members of the opposite sex, but in most cases there was strict sex segregation. Sharing a bath with parents was unusual. Only six individuals remembered sharing a bath with their mothers, while no interviewees remembered bathing with their father.21 The practice of communal bathing stopped well before puberty, usually around the age of ten years, after which strict routines of privacy were enforced. These routines prevented family members seeing each other’s naked bodies, regardless of relationship or sex. Typical in this regard was Ron’s belief that his own mother did not see him ‘without any clothes from roundabout ten years old’. Donna remembered getting undressed in the bedroom that she shared with her five sisters as being ‘like doing the dance of the seven veils’, as they all sought to preserve their modesty.22

It was from within the context of strictly enforced codes of bodily privacy in their familial homes that forty per cent of respondents recalled nudity during bathing in convalescent homes as the most significant deprivation of their privacy, as judged by the number of times it was mentioned and the level of emotional distress recalled. Familial bathing routines were often held in contrast to the institutional nature of convalescent home bathing. But it was only respondents who had been older children, and had stopped sharing baths with their siblings, who believed that their privacy was intruded upon and objected to bathing with their fellow patients. The memories of Annie and Paul illustrate how they both found the bathrooms in their convalescent homes strange, but only ten-year-old Paul objected to sharing a bath with his fellow patients.

Annie (1960, six years old): The bath situation was very strange, and I have this memory of not really knowing what it was at first. Because it was like a great big thing, they’ve got this big thing with all the sinks set around it, it was encased in wood. And then the baths, so many baths that were set into dark wood as well. We had a bath in the night time, (pause) I remember having baths altogether, the girls were separate to the boys like, you’d have a big bath, so you’d all go in and out of these baths, which resulted in much screaming and hilarity.23

Paul (1953, ten years old): [Y]‌ou would all have to have a bath at the same time, you know, not separate, one after the other, but two or three in the bath at the same time, and then scrubbed down by a nurse. But I was, em, self-conscious and refused to get undressed in front of everyone. And, em, I remember that the bathroom area wasn’t very nice, and the first time I looked, well, I had never ever in my life seen anything like, with all these baths lined up along the wall. So, anyway, I objected to this arrangement. I thought I was too old to share a bath, and I didn’t want anyone looking at me in the noddy! So I refused, refused to get undressed.24

Annie’s and Paul’s testimonies demonstrate how privacy associated with bathing was age-dependent, and influenced by normative behaviour from respondents’ familial homes. Older children who were accustomed to bathing separately viewed their bodies as private and personal. Consequently, they expected more privacy than younger children who were accustomed to bathing with their siblings or cousins. Unlike other areas in which respondents tended to assert their agency in oblique, non-confrontational ways, when maintaining their privacy in the bath, respondents were more likely to directly refuse to comply with staff requests. This may reflect the older age of this cohort, who were all over ten years of age, but their testimonies also indicate that it reflected deeply held beliefs that their bodies were private and should not be exposed to others. These beliefs were articulated in the voices of anger and confidence, and demonstrated by the I poem of Penny, who was admitted to a convalescent home for two months in 1958, at the age of eleven years.

I could see them all
I thought what on earth?
I was watching all this going on
I didn’t know where to look
I was mortified
I was confused
I didn’t know what to do
I thought no
I’m not getting in there
I, em, just knew
I could have died
I thought no way
I just knew
I wasn’t going to get undressed
I said no.25

Simon Szreter and Kate Fisher have demonstrated the enforcement of strict codes of bodily privacy by English working-class parents during the first half of the twentieth century.26 They also note a gradual relaxation of inhibitions between some mothers and their young children in the post-war years, an assessment also supported by the memories of six post-war respondents who bathed with their mothers as very young children. It is interesting then, that the interviewee statements examined for this chapter suggest that codes of bodily privacy governing pre-adolescent and teenage nudity were consistent across the period of study. This suggests that although attitudes to adult and younger children’s bodies relaxed, attitudes towards the adolescent body and nudity were enduringly conservative. This observation supports the works of historians who argue that adolescence was viewed as a point in the life cycle associated with psychological turmoil, potential ill health and moral danger.27 This historiography has tended to emphasize attitudes towards adolescent girls, but evidence from interviewees’ statements indicate that the bodies of adolescent boys were considered to be equally at risk.

On the other hand, the oral history testimonies presented in this chapter also indicate that understandings of children’s bodies, bodily functions and associated privacy needs were more complex than can be accommodated by adopting an adolescent watershed model. The age-related privacy codes associated with bathing were at odds with the uniform expectations of privacy during toileting; the inconsistencies in expectations between these two contexts suggests that privacy codes did not simply correlate linearly with age, but rather, were age and context specific.

Memories of diminished privacy did not only relate to individuals’ physical bodies; they also incorporated the need for psychological privacy. Scientific research has indicated that children’s desire for psychological privacy is linked to their developing sense of themselves as individuals, and a need to achieve psychological autonomy by separating themselves from the people and things in their environment.28 Consequently, although many respondents described the institutional environment and regimes as oppressive, only respondents who were over the age of eleven years at the time of their admission recalled wishing for psychological privacy. These respondents emphasized the erosion of their agency by strict daily regimes and their confinement within the authoritative boundaries of homes. The physical boundaries of convalescent homes were shaped by walls, gates and other barriers that limited children’s freedom of movement and confined them within the institutional sphere of authority. This was at odds with the daily experiences of the majority of older patients who recalled playing freely in the streets around their home and in local parks. Mathew Thomson has noted that the culture of outdoor urban freedom continued into the later twentieth century. Pam contrasted the extensive freedom that she was permitted by her parents with the strictly controlled environment of the convalescent home.29

Pam (1948, twelve years old): It was different in them days, we played out in the street all the time, everyone did in them days, all down Burdett Road from Mile End to round Limehouse, and, we’d go on expeditions to the beach by the Tower of London, and there was proper sand, I don’t know if there still is now, and that required a great deal of planning, jam sandwiches, bottles of drink. (laughs) ... I would go to Petticoat Lane, or sometimes Ridley Road [markets] just to have a look around. Em, or sometimes up West, to look in the shops. In the home you weren’t allowed out, sometimes the nurse would take you out, a group of us down to the beach or into the town. But you weren’t allowed out on your own ... In there [convalescent home] everything was done by the clock, same time every day, and the nurses watched what you were doing, (pause) you weren’t allowed to breathe without permission. I found that kinda (pause), em, overwhelming, em, oppressive. I was, like, you were, there wasn’t any freedom. There was a huge gate and when you arrived you drove through this huge gate and then through these huge oak doors, and it was like you, it was a bit like (pause) prison? 30

In remembering the fencing, walls, gates and barriers of their convalescent homes, respondents demonstrated a present-day understanding of their purpose, stating that they ‘were probably there for safety reasons’, ‘to stop small children wandering off’, and to deter ‘unwelcome visitors’.31 Nonetheless, they clearly differentiated between their current understandings and their past feelings of confinement. Thus, the physical boundaries and strict daily regimes, like those described by Pam, prevented children from having time and space in which they were free from supervision. In her study of Australian orphanages, Shurlee Swain noted that children created private spaces in a variety of ways, often breaking or circumventing rules to escape the institutional gaze.32 A similar pattern of exerting agency was described by the older children in this study, who recalled diverse ways in which they created private spaces in the supervised world of convalescent homes. Brenda described how, as an eleven year old, she would hide in her dormitory, and ‘wait for all the other children to leave’, so that she could ‘just be relaxed and daydream’.33 John recalled making a ‘forbidden climb’ into the branches of ‘an old tree and looking over the [convalescent home] wall’, when he felt the need to be alone. Maurice also remembered climbing a tree, but he used the tree as a means of hiding ‘out of sight, away from staff and watch[ing] what everyone was doing’.34

As well as exerting agency by breaking rules, six respondents also used compliance with institutional regimes as a means of achieving a private space. Jean described how she volunteered to help the nurses put away blankets so that she could ‘dawdle, do what I wanted, my own thing’.35 In this way, some children’s compliance was an act of agency, as they consciously adopted behaviour patterns to exercise control and achieve their objectives. Boys and girls were equally likely to use compliance as a means of achieving their objectives, and similarly, both sexes broke rules to achieve privacy. Both modes of behaviour were narrated in the same contrapuntal voice of confidence; moreover, the emergence of the voice of confidence identified continuity of meaning in otherwise disparate and complex behavioural patterns.

To evade institutional surveillance and maintain their privacy during toileting and bathing, and to achieve emotional privacy, children frequently exerted their agency in ways that were oblique or contradictory or did not conform to traditional understandings of agency. Exploring interviewees’ understandings of childhood privacy, and how they sought to maintain this in different situations, has demonstrated the influence of age, relationship and context on both their privacy needs and how individuals responded when these were challenged. The importance of these three factors in mediating children’s responses and their ability to exert agency was also present in their experiences of discipline within convalescent homes.

Discipline

The constant surveillance experienced by children in convalescent homes, and the associated threat to their privacy, was part of a more generalized scheme of control and discipline that shaped their daily experiences. British attitudes towards the precepts and practices of disciplining children have been the subject of a varied and valuable body of historical research.36 Despite this rich and varied historiography, inmate experience and the day-to-day nuances of institutional policies and practices have too commonly been overlooked.

Children of all ages described episodes of discipline in which rules were enforced by nursing staff. In seventy-nine per cent of such cases, respondents characterized their experiences in predominately benign terms. This was especially so when rules were familiar to them and the actions of staff reflected their previous experiences of discipline. As such, most experiences of discipline were not conceived as remarkable or particularly different to that of their familial homes, schools or clubs. Some of the commonly cited examples of benign discipline included rules, such as no running in corridors, no fighting, no pushing, and taking medicine. In most instances, children acquiesced to these rules with no or minimal resistance. Typical in this regard was Patricia who described the daily administration of iron tablets.

Patricia (1947, nine years old): I, em, always tried to avoid taking my medicine. I remember when I had dinner they used to come round and give me a tablet because I was anaemic. It was an iron tablet, and I remember they used to hide it in my dinner, and I can remember eating all round it. Then one of the nurses would notice and she’d shovel it straight in [my mouth] and then inspect to make sure it’d gone down.37

In common with other individuals who recalled these events as relatively minor, Patricia’s characterization corresponded with experiences in her familial homes where taking medicine was a negotiated exchange in which she ultimately complied. Familiarity of experience appeared to increase the acceptability of rules, and Patricia considered the administration of medication in this way to be acceptable, believing that it was the ‘job’ of nursing staff to ensure children took their medicine.

The rule that interviewees recalled contesting most frequently was the requirement to eat all of the food that was served to them. The majority of respondents described the rule being rigidly enforced in their convalescent homes. Chris recalled that staff ‘made a big hoo-hah and song and dance about it, if you dared to leave anything on your plate’.38 However, resistance and refusal to eat unwanted food were relatively common. As Helen’s memories suggest, resistance was frequently covert.

Helen (1954, eight years old): I remember once we had this stew, and I hated stew anyway, had never eaten it, and I didn’t want it, but the nurse stood over me and made me eat it, and I heaved, I retched, oh. (pause) And then, I was eight and it was, to me, it was logical, I would put it in my mouth and then spit it out, drop it under the table and kick it away. Thinking, I suppose that it was going to vanish into thin air or something. But one of the nurses noticed, ‘who’s dropped their food?’ I didn’t confess, I was too frightened to own up.39

In addition to acts of covert disobedience, interviewees recalled engaging with benign discipline in ways that reflected their non-institutional behavioural patterns, including using play, singing and acts of daring that were intended to ridicule and subvert rules. The memories of Dorothy and Michael were typical in this regard.

Dorothy (1946, nine years old): We would make up songs together, about the home and the nuns. I can remember one very, em, much more, she was a very lively girl and I think she was about, must have been a couple of years older than me. And I remember her wrapping a towel around her head, and I thought her very glamorous, and she’d, pretending to be Deanna Durbin, the singing nun, she’d sing (sung to the tune of the nursery rhyme Frère Jacques):

No more talking, no more talking – stand up straight, stand up straight.    

Em, it was something about tripe for tea and then lights out, then

Say a prayer for Jesus, say a prayer to Jesus – God bless you, God bless you!

And it was something like that, and then we’d all join in with the chorus (laughing).

Did the nuns know that you sang songs about them?

Of course not, no! If the nuns were around we were, em, quiet little mice – no more talking and stand up straight! (laughing).40

Michael (1947, ten years old): [A]‌t night time we went to bed early, and the door was locked, we were locked in, when the lights went out you weren’t allowed to talk, and em, if you did speak and the nurses caught you, you got punished, but I don’t know what sort of punishment because I never got caught, not to say I didn’t talk (laughing) ... But we also played dare, when you had to get out of bed, and it’s pitch black mind you, and you had to get out of bed, touch the door handle and say ‘London Bulldog’, and if, if you did, you went up a peg or two, you know, with the other boys (laughing).41

Dorothy and Michael demonstrate the way in which various acts of agency were political in building peer culture. This was not unusual and interviewees’ recollections of benign discipline and how they engaged with such discipline were consistent across the period covered by interviewee experience.42 In their classic study The Lore and Language of School Children, Iona and Peter Opie noted that parody and acts of daring were ways that mid-twentieth-century children exerted independence without having to rebel.43 Correspondingly, although interviewees recalled regularly subverting, or attempting to subvert, benign discipline, they simultaneously described complying with rules with minimal enforcement by staff and without any sense of distress. This was the case for Paul, who recalled that he was treated in a ‘firm but fair way’, and that ‘there was loads of kids, so they needed to keep us under control, but they were kind ladies, and they didn’t mind if we larked about a bit’.44

Despite most respondents recalling that their acts of disobedience were covert and that they eventually acquiesced, in many instances their behaviour also represented powerful examples of agentic behaviour. Rule breaking was not a purely personal action; it built peer culture and increased the political power of individuals within their peer group. This was the case for Frank whose memory of defiance elevated his status among his peers.

Frank (1953, nine years old): You had to do as you are told. But I would sit there, and I would sit there forever, and there was always a nurse there patrolling, they would be monitoring what you ate, how quickly you ate it, no talking, eat up, clean your plate. I remember this one time feeling quite brave; one of the other children had said, the potatoes were like rocks, and they said ‘You can’t throw that potato across the room’, and they were only little potatoes, but they were rock hard. I said ‘I can’, and lobbed it over the other side, very brave on this table of children. Silly experience but it made me feel brave, it gave me confidence that I defied the system (laughs). Anyway, so after that I was the King of the Castle, all because I’d lobbed a potato across the room (laughs).45

Acts of rule breaking that involved an element of daring were important in respondents’ conceptualization of their own agency and power. As demonstrated by Frank’s memory of becoming ‘the King of the Castle’, he was clearly aware that his childhood self had exerted political power, even though as an adult, he believed the act to be a ‘silly experience’.

Just over thirty-five per cent of respondents recalled that they complied with rules to achieve an alternative objective. Chris described how by acquiescing to his weekly laxative medication, he used other children’s resistance to his advantage.

Chris (1950, ten years old): [T]‌hen once a week, a Wednesday, you knew it was syrup of figs day, we always had syrup of figs, if you needed it or not. We’d stand in a big, long line around the table upstairs in the bedroom, and they’d shove the same spoon, everybody had the same spoon, they’d just shove it in your mouth – it was horrible stuff! And then there would be some kids refused to open their mouth, or wouldn’t swallow it, you know, all that. But, you see, I’d got it worked out, I soon realised that when we got dosed up with, em, syrup of figs it was also bread and dripping night. They put out jugs of hot milk or coco or malt, and bread and dripping, and I loved that, that bread and dripping was just lovely. (laughs) And you see, I was on to this, and I knew that I could get down there first if I had the syrup of figs quick.46

In narrating accounts of compliance and receiving some form of advantage, the voice of confidence came to prominence. In this way, some children’s compliance was an act of agency, as they consciously used their consent to treatment as a form of exercising control. Moreover, for these children, giving their consent to an unpleasant treatment actually increased their sense of power and control.

Interviewees’ recollections of benign discipline and the ways in which they engaged with such discipline was consistent across the period covered by interviewee experience.47 Although resistance to rules was relatively common, accounts of physical punishment by nursing staff were very rare, with only three cases described by respondents. In comparison, ninety-six per cent of interviewees described receiving physical punishment from their parents, and seventy-two per cent recalled physical punishment as a common form of discipline at school. Low levels of physical punishment in the homes suggest an atmosphere of relative tolerance. There were, however, a significant number of events in which staff used humiliation as a form of discipline. Most of these events involved episodes of incontinence, usually nocturnal enuresis.

Historically, attitudes to nocturnal enuresis were intolerant, and punitive measures were believed to be an effective treatment.48 Interviewee experiences reflected prevailing beliefs and attitudes, and many recalled ‘treatments’ that involved public shaming, washing their own sheets, having their noses rubbed in wet sheets and being left with wet bedding or in wet clothes all night. The testimonies of Jean, Rosamund and Annie reveal the strength of trauma experienced from such humiliating punishments, and provide poignant witness to its lasting effect on individuals.

Jean (1938, eleven years old): And one night I remember coughing. I had phlegm and I was trying I was trying to cough it up, but I think it was all the coughing, and I wet the bed, and, em, (pause) I started to cry. And the night nurse came and pulled me out of bed, shouting at me, and made me stand in the bathroom for hours, in the middle of the night, till I’d dried out. (pause) Not something you want to remember.49

Rosamund (1956, nine years old): I remember standing at the end of the bed with, em, a wet sheet that had stayed wet from the previous night, and hold it as a punishment, ah, with a nun being in her little room at the end of the dormitory until I dropped, until I dropped to sleep standing up, and they took it away from me, because that should teach the children not to wet the bed. But we’ve got it in our family, I think it’s a bit hereditary or, em, it’s just children, I don’t know. Ah, I have obviously been through this an awful lot inside, to myself, so I’m not crying my eyes out, I’m crying inside, still crying inside.50

Annie (1960, six years old): I wet the bed most nights and the nurses ridiculed me. They, em, (very long pause) they dealt with it by punishing me, em, they would rub my nose in the wet sheet (pause), and put a ribbon on the end of my bed, so everyone knew I wet the bed. (clears throat) The nurses were not very kind to me, and those, those memories have stayed with me all these years, they don’t leave you. Do you know that?51

In total, nine respondents recalled being punished for nocturnal enuresis; their ages ranged from six to eleven years old. As with other accounts of discipline involving humiliation, interviewees articulated a sense of injustice at their treatment. Their accounts were narrated entirely in the voice of despondency, frequently exhibiting signs of discomposure, moving backwards and forwards between events and in chronology. In recalling accounts of humiliation, respondents appeared to have been overwhelmed by their experience and unable to exert a sense of agency or power. Children’s lack of agency was related to an overwhelming sense of isolation caused by humiliating punishments that divided them from their peers and undermined any sense of control. The following extract from the I poem of Martin recalls the experience of lining up to have his underwear inspected for marks, and demonstrates that in certain circumstances children appeared to have very little agency.

I don’t know why she did it
I was told off loudly in front of everyone
I think it was
I had to show her my underwear to see if there were any, em, kind of, of skid marks
I was eight
I didn’t have anyone to turn to
I had a lot of marks
I was humiliated in front of everyone
I had to stand in a corner on my own
I was told off a lot
I’ve never mentioned this to anyone
I just mention it to you because I was just a boy
I remember crying
I was crying
I mean she just humiliated me in front of everybody
I don’t know what
I didn’t have anyone
I don’t know how I coped with it. Oh God.
I just don’t know.52

The total absence of power in Martin’s account was typical of individuals’ memories of discipline that involved humiliation – even among respondents who recalled acts of agency in other circumstances. Whereas, in most forms of agentic behaviour interviewees recalled building and drawing on support from peer culture, this support appears to have been dissolved by humiliation. In their studies of boarding school children, Vyvyen Brendon and Joy Schaverien have noted similar feelings of helplessness associated with humiliating punishments.53 W. R. Meyer’s study of day-pupils in Leeds has demonstrated that teachers who humiliated pupils were ‘likely to lead to umbrage taking and protest’ by parents.54 This indicates that pupils informed their parents about humiliating punishments, and, as such, they exerted agency through their familial support network. Hence, the prolonged physical separation of children from their families in convalescent homes combined with the isolation caused by humiliating punishments rendered interviewees unable to challenge certain forms of discipline, even when they perceived them to be unjust.

It is, then, significant that, in common with boarding schools, children in convalescent homes were separated from their families by distance and restrictive visiting practices. The continuation of restricted visiting by homes and the prolonged separation of children from their parents is at odds with the practice of children’s hospitals that, influenced by John Bowlby’s and James Robertson’s work on maternal separation, had generally introduced daily open visiting for parents by the mid-1950s.55 The explanation for the marked difference in policies between hospitals and homes is unclear, but entries in official records continued to stress the importance of providing children with fresh air, good food, rest and respite from the overcrowding and pollution of London, without discussing their emotional well-being. The primacy afforded to physical health over mental health points to an area of tension in children’s healthcare, where new concepts and understandings of children’s emotional needs and child psychology collided and competed with traditional ideas of childcare and medicine.

Conclusion

As part of broader schemes of welfare reforms, children’s healthcare provision changed during the mid twentieth century from that of a mixed economy of philanthropic and local authority sponsored endeavours to a central government sponsored National Health Service.56 Yet, throughout this transition, institutional convalescent care for children was an enduring and accepted part of medical orthodoxy. Although aspects of children’s convalescent care were often conceptually dynamic, the day-to-day experiences of child patients were remarkably stable throughout the nineteenth and twentieth centuries. This demonstrates that new understandings of child psychology did not permeate all areas of children’s institutional care.

Interviewees’ testimonies revealed that the significance of children’s convalescence extended beyond its role in consolidating biological recovery, and generated acute and often long-term emotional responses. Enmeshed within interviewees’ narratives were a number of events relating to toileting, bathing and discipline that occurred over and over again, from one interviewee to the next. The significance of these events to an individual was clear to observe. Their diffusion through the majority of testimonies indicated that they were essential to our understanding of children’s experience of convalescent homes and institutional care more broadly. What was less clear was how a group of disparate events were related. An analysis of interviewees’ testimonies using Gilligan’s Listening Guide demonstrated that what appeared to be separate events were a chain of remembered experiences in which interviewees exerted or attempted to exert agency. The varying ability with which individuals exerted agency draws attention to the importance of age, context and relationships in children’s ability to exert power.

Through the themes of privacy and discipline, it has been possible to interrogate episodes of childhood agency within an institutional setting. Children’s power in their relationships with adult carers cannot be explained through a binary of adult power versus child resistance. Instead, children exerted agency in a myriad of ways, including negotiation, resistance, compliance, play and peer group activity. Moreover, respondents’ testimonies indicate that they understood themselves to be active participants in exerting autonomy, but they exerted their power in distinctly age-specific ways that do not conform to many of the ordinary understandings of adult autonomy and power.

Resistance or compliance with institutional regimes was directly related to children’s experiences in their familial homes. Experiences that were familiar to children were more likely to be complied with. Conversely, practices that were unfamiliar or perceived to be unjust were frequently resisted, albeit often covertly. And practices that contravened privacy norms were likely to be overtly resisted, particularly when associated with adolescent and pre-adolescent nudity. Although most memories of resistance were covert, they often represented powerful agentic behaviour by building peer culture and individual status. Peer groups were an important site and medium of childhood agency across the period covered by interviewee experience. However, disciplinary methods employed by staff that involved the use of public humiliation appear to have dissolved peer group support and resulted in a corresponding absence of agency. Respondents’ memories draw into focus the pernicious effect of isolation and underscore the detrimental effect of the physical separation of children from their families. The significance of this observation extends beyond the study of children’s convalescent homes and points towards broader experiences of children’s institutional care in historical and contemporary settings.

For historians, the need to engage with subject populations on their own terms is a well-established tenet. However, the challenge for historians of childhood is to use methodologies that capture meaning that may be obscured by an adult-centric bias. While historians of gender, race and sexuality have done much to extend definitions of agency, questions of how children historically enacted power require yet further discrimination. Indeed, the oral history testimonies examined in this chapter suggest that there is not a single model of childhood agency; rather, there are many agencies that are age-specific, relational and contextual. As such, it is necessary for scholars to think about age in more complex ways, beyond the static or linear categories of childhood and adulthood.

1Royston, C31MM-R1. To maintain anonymity, all of the interviewees were assigned a unique identifying number and their names changed. Interviewees’ age and year of admission are detailed next to their name.

2For welfare clinics and domiciliary nurse visits, see: V. Fildes, L. Marks and H. Marland, ed., Women and Children First: International Maternal and Infant Welfare 1870–1945 (London, 1992); H. Marland, ‘A pioneer in infant welfare: the Huddersfield scheme 1903–1920’, The Society for the Social History of Medicine, vi (1993), 25–49; L. Marks, Metropolitan Maternity: Maternal and Infant Welfare Services in Early Twentieth Century London (Amsterdam, 1996). For school medical inspections, see: J. Welshman, The School Medical Service in England and Wales, 1907–1939 (Oxford, 1989); B. Harris, The Health of the Schoolchild: a History of the School Medical Service in England and Wales (Buckingham, 1995).

3H. Hendrick, ‘Children’s emotional well-being and mental health in early post-Second World War Britain’, in Cultures of Child Health in Britain and the Netherlands in the Twentieth Century, ed. M. Gijswijt-Hofstra and H. Marland (Amsterdam, 2003), pp. 213–42, at pp. 219–21.

4S. Mumm, ‘“Not worse than other girls”: the convent-based rehabilitation of fallen women in Victorian Britain’, Journal of Social History, xxix (1996), 527–46; L. Abrams, The Orphan Country: Children of Scotland’s Broken Homes from 1845 to the Present Day (Edinburgh, 1998), pp. 164, 249, 252; L. Murdoch, Imagined Orphans: Poor Families, Child Welfare, and Contested Citizenship in London (New Brunswick, 2006), pp. 1–11, 12–43; L. Peters, Orphan Texts: Victorian Orphans, Culture and Empire (Manchester, 2000), pp. 8–9; S. Swain and M. Hillel, Child, Nation, Race and Empire: Child Rescue Discourse, England, Canada and Australia, 1850–1915 (Manchester, 2010), pp. 129–30.

5C. Newman, ‘To punish or protect: the new Poor Law and the English workhouse’, International Journal of Historical Archaeology, xviii (2014), 122–45; G. Frost, Victorian Childhoods (London, 2009), p. 123; J. Hamlett, L. Hoskins and R. Preston, ed., Residential Institutions in Britain, 1725–1970 (London, 2013); J. Hamlett and L. Hoskins, ‘Comfort in small things? Clothing, control and agency in county lunatic asylums in nineteenth- and early twentieth-century England’, Journal of Victorian Culture, xviii (2013), 93–114; C. Soares, ‘A “permanent environment of brightness, warmth, and ‘homeliness’”: domesticity and authority in a Victorian children’s institution’, Journal of Victorian Culture, xxiii (2018), 1–24.

6A. Portelli, ‘What makes oral history different’, in The Oral History Reader, ed. R. Perks and A. Thomson (London, 1998), pp. 63–74.

7C. Gilligan, In A Different Voice: Psychological Theory and Women’s Development (Harvard, 1982). Originally designed for the analysis of contemporary self-narratives, it has been effectively deployed by Laura Tisdall to analyse autobiographies of children who were in long-term institutional care between 1918 and 1946; and by Florence Sutcliffe-Braithwaite to analyse a single oral history transcript collected by psychologists in the early 1980s. L. Tisdall, ‘“That was what life in Bridgeburn had made her”: reading the autobiographies of children in institutional care in England, 1918–46’, Twentieth Century British History, xxiv (2013), 351–75; F. Sutcliffe-Braithwaite, ‘New perspectives from unstructured interviews: young women, gender, and sexuality on the Isle of Sheppey in 1980’, SAGE Open, vi (2016), 1–11.

8Chris, C18MM-C1.

9M. J. Maynes, ‘Age as a category of historical analysis: history, agency, and narratives of childhood’, The Journal of the History of Childhood and Youth, i (2008), 114–24.

10L. Davidoff and C. Hall, Family Fortunes: Men and Women of the English Middle-class, 1780–1850 (Chicago, 1987) pp. 357–97; D. Webb, Privacy and Solitude in the Middle Ages (London, 2007); D. Vincent, I Hope I Don’t Intrude: Privacy and its Dilemmas in Nineteenth-Century Britain (Oxford, 2015); D. Vincent, Privacy: a Short History (Cambridge, 2016).

11R. S. Laufer and M. Wolfe, ‘Privacy as a concept and a social issue: a multidimensional developmental theory’, Journal of Social Issues, xxxiii (1977), 22–42; R. Parke and D. Sawin, ‘The family in early infancy: social interactional and attitudinal analyses’, in The Father-Infant Relationship: Observational Studies in a Family Context, ed. F. A. Pedersen (New York, 1980), 44–70; S. Petronio, Boundaries of Privacy: Dialectics of Disclosure (New York, 2002), pp. 73–5; L. S. Shapiro and G. Margolin, ‘Growing up wired: social networking sites and adolescent psychosocial development’, Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, xvii (2014), 1–18.

12Petronio, Boundaries of Privacy, pp. 73–5.

13Anthony, C43MM-A1.

14Bertha, C2MM-B1.

15Mavis, C1MM-M1.

16W. Corsaro, The Sociology of Childhood (Thousand Oaks, 1997), pp. 147–52; W. Corsaro, Friendship and Peer Culture in the Early Years (Westpoint, 1985), pp. 301–15.

17Saul, C9MM-S1.

18Dora, C51MM-D1.

19Corsaro, The Sociology of Childhood, p. 151.

20Pam, C44MM-P1.

21David, C30MM-D1; George, C36MM-G1; Annie, C25MM-M1.

22Ron, C48MM-R1; Donna, C19MM-D1.

23Annie, C25MM-A1.

24Paul, C41MM-P1.

25Penny, C10MM-P1.

26S. Szreter and K. Fisher, Sex Before the Sexual Revolution: Intimate life in England 1918–1963 (Cambridge, 2010), pp. 271–7.

27J.-M. Strange, ‘The assault on ignorance: teaching menstrual etiquette in England, c.1920s to 1960s’, Social History of Medicine, xiv (2001), 247–65; V. Long and H. Marland, ‘From danger and motherhood to health and beauty: health advice for the factory girl in early twentieth-century Britain’, Twentieth Century British History, xx (2009), 454–81; H. Marland, Health and Girlhood in Britain, 1874–1920 (Basingstoke, 2013), pp. 25–53; J. J. Brunberg, The Body Project: an Intimate History of American Girls (New York, 2010); K. Fisher and S. Toulalan, Bodies, Sex and Desire from the Renaissance to the Present (Basingstoke, 2011).

28J. Piaget and B. Inhelder, The Psychology of the Child (New York, 1969); C. J. Weigel-Garrey, C. Cook and M. Brotherson, ‘Children and privacy: choice, control, and access in home environments’, Journal of Family Issues, xix (1998), 42–64; K. D. McKinney, ‘Space, body, and mind: parental perceptions of children’s privacy needs’, Journal of Family Issues, xix (1998), 75–100.

29M. Thomson, Lost Freedom: the Landscape of the Child and the British Post-War Settlement (Oxford, 2013).

30Pam, C44MM-P1.

31Martin, C3MM-B1; Theresa, C37MM-T1; Maria, C42MM-M1.

32P. Hewitt, The Looked After Kid (London, 2014); S. Swain, ‘Institutionalized childhood: the orphanage remembered’, The Journal of the History of Childhood and Youth, viii (2015), 17–33.

33Brenda, C53MM-B1.

34John, C17MM-J1; Maurice, C24MM-M1.

35Jean, C35MM-J1.

36S. Humphries, Hooligans or Rebels? An Oral History of Working-Class Childhood and Youth, 1889–1939 (Oxford, 1981); Murdoch, Imagined Orphans, pp. 120–42; H. Shore, ‘Punishment, reformation, or welfare responses to “the problem” of juvenile crime in Victorian and Edwardian Britain’, in Punishment and Control in Historical Perspective, ed. H. Shore (Basingstoke, 2008), pp. 158–75; D. Thom, ‘“Beating children is wrong”: Domestic life, psychological thinking and the permissive turn’, in The Politics of Domestic Authority in Britain since 1800, ed. L. Delap, B. Griffin and A. Wills (Basingstoke, 2009), pp. 261–83; J. Middleton, ‘The experience of corporal punishment in schools, 1890–1940’, History of Education, xxxvii (2008), 253–75; A. Wills, ‘Resistance, identity and historical change in residential institutions for juvenile delinquents, 1920–1950’, in Punishment and Control in Historical Perspective, ed. H. Johnston (Basingstoke, 2008), pp. 215–34.

37Patricia, C5MM-P1.

38Chris, C18MM-C1.

39Helen, C39MM-M1.

40Dorothy, C2MM-D1.

41Michael, C6MM-M1.

42Dorothy, C2MM-D1; Sybil, C55MM-S2. For a discussion of children’s singing games, especially the role of mimicry, see: I. Opie and P. Opie, The Singing Game (Oxford, 1988), pp. 286–310. For a discussion of daring games, see: I. Opie and P. Opie, Children’s Games and Street and Playground (Oxford, 1984), pp. 263–72.

43I. Opie and P. Opie, The Lore and Language of School Children (Oxford, 1959), pp. 86, 377–8.

44Paul, C41MM-P1.

45Frank, C47MM-F1.

46Chris, C18MM-C1.

47Dorothy, C2MM-D1; Sybil, C55MM-S2.

48‘The education of educators’, BMJ, ii (1900), 1457; D. M. Odlum, ‘Nocturnàl enuresis’, BMJ, i (1940), 8–10; J. Malloy and A. G. Bodman, ‘Enuresis’, BMJ, i (1940), 108–9; I. Gordan, ‘Allergy, enuresis, and stammering’, BMJ, i (1942), 357–8; J. J. Michaels and A. Steinberg, ‘Persistent enuresis and juvenile delinquency’, The British Journal of Delinquency, iii (1952), 114–23; J. Crane, ‘Rethinking how evacuees influenced post-war British thinking on health’, Retrospectives, ii (2013), 22–41.

49Jean, C35MM-J1.

50Rosamund, C32MM-R1.

51Annie, C25MM-A1.

52Martin, C3MM-B1.

53V. Brendon, Prep School Children: a Class Apart Over Two Centuries (London, 2009), pp. 141, 170; J. Schaverien, Boarding School Syndrome: the Psychological Trauma of the ‘Privileged’ Child (London, 2015), pp. 168, 16.

54W. R. Meyer, ‘School vs. parent in Leeds, 1902–1944’, Journal of Educational Administration and History, xx (1990), 16–26.

55J. Robertson and J. Bowlby, ‘Responses of young children to separation from their mothers II: observations of the sequences of response of children aged 18 to 24 months during the course of separation’, Courrier du Centre International de l’Enfance, iii (1952), 131–42; J. Robertson, ‘A two-year-old goes to hospital’, Concord Video and Film Council (1952); J. Robertson, Young Children in Hospital (London, 1958), pp. 1–20; J. Robertson, ‘Going to hospital with mother’, Concord Video and Film Council (1958).

For a discussion of visiting policies on children’s wards, see: Hendrick, ‘Children’s emotional well-being and mental health in early post-Second World War Britain’, pp. 219–21.

56C. Webster, ed., Caring for Health: History and Diversity (Milton Keynes, 1993); B. Harris, The Origins of the British Welfare State: Society, State and Social Welfare in England and Wales, 1800–1945 (Basingstoke, 2004); R. Lowe, The Welfare State in Britain since 1945 (Basingstoke, 2005).

Annotate

Next Chapter
6. ‘The Borough Council have done a great deal ... I hope they continue to do so in the future’: children, community and the welfare state, 1941–55
PreviousNext
All rights reserved
Powered by Manifold Scholarship. Learn more at
Opens in new tab or windowmanifoldapp.org